There's a moment in every L&D professional's career when they realise that the mind isn't a black box. It happens differently for everyone, but the recognition is always the same: learning isn't just about inputs and outputs, rewards and punishments. Something fascinating and complex is happening between the ears, and if we're serious about improving performance, we need to understand what that something is.
This recognition drove significant changes in psychology during the 1950s and 1960s, fundamentally reshaping how we think about learning and development. Yet for all its influence on our field, cognitivism remains one of the most misunderstood and misapplied learning theories in corporate training. We've embraced its language whilst often missing its substance, adopted its techniques whilst ignoring its principles, and claimed its authority whilst discarding its insights.
The mind strikes back
To understand cognitivism's impact, you need to appreciate what it was responding to. During the first half of the 20th century, behaviourism dominated psychology and, by extension, corporate training. The mind was considered unknowable and therefore irrelevant. Learning was simply a matter of conditioning responses through carefully managed stimuli and reinforcement schedules.
This approach worked well enough for basic skill development and compliance training, but by the 1950s, its limitations were becoming impossible to ignore. The emerging field of computer science was demonstrating that complex information processing was possible, the study of language was revealing sophisticated cognitive structures, and practitioners were finding that behaviourist approaches simply couldn't explain or address many workplace learning challenges (Gardner, 1987).
The cognitive approach emerged from this frustration, driven by researchers who insisted that understanding mental processes was not only possible but essential for effective learning design. George Miller's 1956 paper "The Magical Number Seven" demonstrated that the mind had measurable limitations and structures that could be studied scientifically (Miller, 1956). Suddenly, the black box had windows, and we could begin to see what was happening inside.
Note: This research has since been challenged, with the ‘magic number’ reducing syudy by study (I believe we are currenlt at 3-5). But the movement remainds sound, I think.
The pioneers who opened the box
Jean Piaget laid crucial groundwork by demonstrating that learning involves active construction of knowledge rather than passive absorption of information. His research revealed that people don't simply accumulate facts but build increasingly sophisticated mental models through interaction with their environment (Piaget, 1977). For L&D, this meant that effective training needed to engage learners in active knowledge construction rather than information transmission.
Jerome Bruner extended these insights by exploring how people discover and organise knowledge. His work on scaffolding showed that learning could be accelerated through carefully structured support that was gradually removed as competence developed (Bruner, 1966). This principle changed instructional design, introducing the idea that training should provide temporary frameworks that learners could internalise and eventually discard.
David Ausubel contributed perhaps the most practical insight with his famous assertion that "the most important single factor influencing learning is what the learner already knows" (Ausubel, 1968). This seemingly obvious statement reshaped training design by emphasising the critical importance of prior knowledge assessment and the need to connect new learning to existing mental structures.
Lev Vygotsky introduced the concept of the Zone of Proximal Development, demonstrating that learning occurs most effectively when challenges are just beyond current capability but within reach with appropriate support (Vygotsky, 1978). This insight provided a framework for calibrating training difficulty and understanding when learners need additional scaffolding.
The corporate adoption
Corporate training embraced cognitivism with considerable enthusiasm. Instructional design models proliferated, each claiming to optimise cognitive processing and enhance knowledge construction.
This is also where a lot of junk such as learning styles, multiple intelligences etc., really took off. Whilst these don’t fully invalidate the ideas behind cognitivism, they do highlight the danger of such rapid, enthusiastic adoption of science we don’t actually understand… remind you of a certain technology?
The results were genuinely significant. Management development programmes began focusing on strategic thinking rather than just behavioural compliance. Case study methodologies flourished as trainers recognised that learners needed to actively process complex scenarios rather than simply memorise procedures. Problem-based learning emerged as organisations realised that employees needed to develop thinking skills, not just perform tasks.
The emphasis on mental models improved technical training. Instead of teaching procedures by rote, programmes began helping learners understand underlying principles and conceptual frameworks. This approach proved particularly effective for complex systems training, where understanding the logic behind procedures enabled more flexible and adaptive performance (Anderson et al., 2001).
Research consistently demonstrates cognitivism's effectiveness for developing higher-order thinking skills. Studies show that cognitive approaches achieve effect sizes of 0.71 for problem-solving training and 0.68 for strategic thinking development, significantly outperforming behaviourist methods for these complex capabilities (Arthur et al., 2003).
Where we lost the plot
Despite these successes, corporate cognitivism often devolved into what might charitably be called "cognitive theatre", the appearance of sophisticated learning design without the substance. The most visible manifestation of this decline was the wholesale adoption of learning styles theory, despite mounting evidence that matching instruction to supposed learning preferences produces no measurable benefits (Pashler et al., 2008).
The misapplication of multiple intelligences theory created similar problems. Howard Gardner's nuanced framework for understanding different types of intelligence was reduced to simplistic training categories that promised to address "visual learners" and "kinaesthetic learners" through superficial design modifications rather than genuine cognitive engagement (Gardner, 2006).
Perhaps more damaging was the embrace of "discovery learning" approaches that misinterpreted cognitive principles to justify minimal guidance and unstructured exploration. Research consistently shows that discovery learning is particularly ineffective for novice learners, who lack the prior knowledge necessary to benefit from unguided exploration (Kirschner et al., 2006). Yet this approach became synonymous with "cognitive training" in many organisations.
Note: Discovery learning can be a powerful tool, but is often deployed in the very worst scenarios. I have seen it used in many onboarding programs, exatly the wrong time to be expecting people to engage in this kind of expeirence.
The focus on engagement over effectiveness created additional problems. Cognitive approaches were often evaluated based on learner satisfaction rather than knowledge acquisition or performance improvement. This led to the proliferation of entertaining but ineffective training that stimulated cognitive activity without building cognitive capability.
What cognitivism actually teaches us
When properly understood and applied, cognitivism offers powerful principles for designing learning experiences that genuinely improve performance. The key insights remain as relevant today as they were sixty years ago.
Working memory has severe limitations that must be respected in training design. Miller's original research showing that people can typically hold only seven (plus or minus two) items in conscious awareness has been refined to suggest even stricter limits, particularly for complex information (Cowan, 2001). This means that effective training must carefully manage cognitive load, presenting information in digestible chunks and avoiding unnecessary complexity.
Prior knowledge is the foundation of all new learning. Ausubel's insight requires that training begins with genuine assessment of what learners already know, not what we assume they should know based on job titles or experience levels. Building connections between new information and existing knowledge structures accelerates learning and improves retention (Bransford et al., 2000).
Active processing is essential for knowledge construction. Simply presenting information, regardless of how clearly or engagingly, cannot create the mental models necessary for effective performance. Learners must actively manipulate, analyse, and apply new information to integrate it with existing knowledge structures.
Scaffolding enables learners to achieve beyond their current capabilities. Bruner's insights about temporary support structures remain crucial for training design. Effective programmes provide frameworks, templates, and guidance that learners can use whilst developing independent capability, then gradually remove these supports as competence increases.
Practical applications for modern L&D
Understanding cognitive principles enables more sophisticated and effective training design across every category of workplace learning.
For complex skill development, cognitive approaches emphasise building mental models before developing procedural fluency. Rather than drilling procedures until they become automatic, effective training helps learners understand the principles underlying effective performance. This creates more flexible and adaptive capabilities that transfer better to novel situations.
In leadership development, cognitive frameworks excel at developing strategic thinking and decision-making capabilities. Case study methodologies, scenario planning exercises, and structured reflection activities all leverage cognitive principles to build the mental models necessary for effective leadership performance.
For problem-solving training, cognitive approaches focus on developing thinking strategies rather than domain-specific solutions. Teaching frameworks for problem analysis, solution generation, and decision evaluation creates transferable capabilities that improve performance across multiple contexts.
Technical training benefits from cognitive emphasis on conceptual understanding. Rather than memorising step-by-step procedures, learners develop understanding of system logic and underlying principles. This approach creates more robust knowledge that withstands system changes and supports troubleshooting when procedures don't work as expected.
The measurement challenge
Cognitivism creates particular challenges for training evaluation because its effects often involve invisible mental processes rather than observable behaviours. Traditional training metrics focused on behaviour change and performance outcomes may miss important cognitive developments that enable future learning and adaptation.
Effective evaluation of cognitive training requires assessment of mental models, not just task performance. This might involve concept mapping exercises, explanation tasks, or transfer problems that require applying learned principles to novel situations. These approaches are more complex than traditional testing but provide insight into whether training has actually built the cognitive structures necessary for sustained performance improvement.
Long-term evaluation becomes particularly important because cognitive training often shows delayed effects as learners integrate new knowledge structures with existing expertise. The benefits may not appear immediately but compound over time as learners apply their enhanced thinking capabilities to increasingly complex challenges.
Neuroscience validation
Modern neuroscience has largely validated cognitive psychology's insights about learning, whilst providing additional detail about the mechanisms involved. Brain imaging studies confirm that learning involves physical changes in neural networks, supporting cognitivism's emphasis on active knowledge construction (Draganski et al., 2004).
Research on neuroplasticity demonstrates that the brain continues to form new connections throughout life, validating adult learning approaches based on cognitive principles (Kempermann, 2019). The discovery of mirror neurons has provided new insights into how observational learning works, extending Bandura's social cognitive theory with biological evidence (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004).
Cognitive load research has been refined through neuroscience studies that show how working memory limitations affect learning. This research supports sophisticated instructional design approaches that manage intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load to optimise learning effectiveness (Sweller et al., 2019).
The integration opportunity
The future of effective L&D lies not in choosing between cognitive approaches and other learning theories, but in understanding when and how to integrate cognitive principles with behavioural, social, and constructivist insights.
Cognitive approaches excel at building thinking capabilities and mental models but may be less effective for developing automatic behavioural responses or collaborative skills. The most sophisticated training programmes combine cognitive frameworks for understanding with behavioural techniques for skill development and social approaches for application and refinement.
This integration requires understanding the specific contribution that cognitive approaches make to performance improvement. When employees need to develop problem-solving capabilities, strategic thinking skills, or conceptual understanding, cognitive methods provide the most direct path to effectiveness. When they need to develop routine procedures or collaborative capabilities, other approaches may be more appropriate.
My honest assessment
Sixty-five years after cognitive psychology emerged, what can we say about its contribution to L&D practice? The evidence suggests both remarkable successes and persistent failures in application.
Cognitivism succeeded in demonstrating that the mind matters. Training programmes that ignore cognitive processes, working memory limitations, and prior knowledge consistently underperform those that respect these factors. The emphasis on active learning and knowledge construction has improved training effectiveness across multiple domains.
The approach transformed management and leadership development by providing frameworks for developing strategic thinking capabilities that behaviourist approaches couldn't address. Case study methodologies, scenario planning, and structured reflection activities all emerged from cognitive insights and continue to show strong effectiveness.
However, cognitive approaches have been frequently misapplied through superficial adoption of buzzwords without understanding of underlying principles. Learning styles, discovery learning, and engagement-focused design have often undermined rather than enhanced cognitive effectiveness.
The path forward requires more sophisticated understanding of when and how to apply cognitive principles, combined with rigorous evaluation of their effectiveness in specific training contexts. Cognitivism offers powerful tools for improving performance, but only when applied with the same intellectual rigour that created the theory in the first place.
Cognitivism's greatest contribution may be its insistence that learning involves thinking, and that effective training must respect and support the cognitive processes that enable performance improvement. In an era where artificial intelligence is transforming work itself, this emphasis on developing human thinking capabilities becomes not just valuable but essential.
How might you apply cognitive principles to enhance a training programme you're currently designing? What would change if you focused on building mental models rather than delivering content?
References
Anderson, L. W., Krathwohl, D. R., Airasian, P., Cruikshank, K., Mayer, R., Pintrich, P., Raths, J. & Wittrock, M. (2001). A taxonomy for learning, teaching and assessing: A revision of Bloom's taxonomy of educational objectives. Longman.
Arthur, W., Bennett, W., Edens, P. S. & Bell, S. T. (2003). Effectiveness of training in organizations: a meta-analysis of design and evaluation features. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(2), 234-245.
Ausubel, D. P. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L. & Cocking, R. R. (Eds.). (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school. National Academy Press.
Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Harvard University Press.
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87-114.
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U. & May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427(6972), 311-312.
Gardner, H. (1987). The mind's new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. Basic Books.
Gardner, H. (2006). Multiple intelligences: New horizons in theory and practice. Basic Books.
Kempermann, G. (2019). Environmental enrichment, new neurons and the neurobiology of individuality. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(4), 235-245.
Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J. & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75-86.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81-97.
Pashler, H., McDaniel, M., Rohrer, D. & Bjork, R. (2008). Learning styles: Concepts and evidence. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9(3), 105-119.
Piaget, J. (1977). The development of thought: Equilibration of cognitive structures. Viking Press.
Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169-192.
Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J. & Paas, F. (2019). Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educational Psychology Review, 31(2), 261-292.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.
This is a nice summary. However, I am unaware of any practical application in workplace training of the theory mirror neurons. At least nothing beyond what cognitive science and social science tell us.